The Influence of Outdoor Play on Social and Cognitive Development


Macquarie University, Sydney, Australia

PDF version

Introduction

There is converging evidence that repeated exposure to high quality, unstructured outdoor play opportunities has a positive impact on social and cognitive development. Quality outdoor play can present difficulties for parents and educators who may be used to high levels of supervision and instruction.1  

Subject

There has been a recent trend for educational contexts to reduce the amount of time children spend in unstructured outdoor play due to increased pressure for more direct instruction.2 Simultaneously, heightened risk aversion,3 even outside of educational settings, results in fewer opportunities for independent outdoor play.4 

Problems

The impact of reductions in opportunities for unstructured outdoor play may already be apparent5 and responsible for declines in creative thinking,6 reduced ability to get along with others7 and increased psychopathology.7  

The majority of research has been conducted with pre-schoolers or school-aged children. Little is known about younger children.

Research Context

A significant focus of outdoor play research has been related to health outcomes, particularly factors such as physical activity and obesity that can be associated with lifelong chronic illness.8,5 There has been less research on cognitive and social outcomes and there is a lack of large scale high quality studies.  Nonetheless, this is a growing area of research that includes researchers from multiple disciplines using a wide range of methods ranging from ethnographic studies to randomised controlled trials.

Key Research Questions

Major research questions in this area include:

  • What is the relationship between outdoor play and cognitive development?
  • What is the relationship between outdoor play and social development?
  • Can outdoor play interventions help improve cognition and/or social development?

Recent Research Results

Outdoor play is thought to have an impact on physiological factors that underpin cognitive and social development. The strongest evidence relates to motor9 and visual development.10 Less is known about the development of the brain although studies from nonhuman species have found that play deprivation has a negative impact on cortical development.11

The most promising research area involves the investigation of outdoor play and executive functions (EFs). EFs include cognitive processes such as inhibitory control, working memory and cognitive flexibility and are associated with positive academic and social outcomes.12 There is evidence that aerobic exercise improves EFs13 and this would perhaps account for the relationship with outdoor play.  However aerobic exercise alone does not seem to be sufficient. The physical activity that is associated with aerobic exercise needs to be in the context of complexity, novelty and diversity to promote EFs.14 Research on pretend play and EFs has been found to be promising15 as has the Tools of the Mind curriculum which incorporates pretend play.16 While it is reasonable to assume that outdoor play involving physical activity and pretend play would be ideal for promoting EFs, the critical studies are yet to be conducted.

Human geographers and environmental psychologists have examined spatial understandings, in particular, how children remember and understand larger environments, such as neighbourhoods. Studies have identified associations between active travel/independent mobility and children’s spatial knowledge.17,18 Primary school aged children have demonstrated better knowledge of home-school routes and objects encountered than children travelling in motorised vehicles. Independence of travel, way-finding and speed of travel (i.e., slow to observe details) have been considered important in developing children’s spatial knowledge.17 A recent qualitative study of children’s active travel to school found participants engaged in playful experiences including careful risk-taking, trying new routes, changing their journeys in increments and experimenting with new activities.19 These children were also found to show increased levels of social engagement and responsibilities such as looking out for peers and siblings.

One method of changing the outdoor play environment in primary schools has been to introduce loose parts play (LPP).8,20 LPP generally involves introduction of large objects with no obvious play purpose onto the playground.21 A systematic review indicated promise in the LPP approach to support social-emotional development, but more high quality work is needed and sensitivity of outcome measures could be improved.22 LPP research has generated some unexpected findings. For example, in a recent randomised controlled trial, children in the intervention group (i.e., had opportunities for LPP) were more likely to say they had been pushed/shoved on the playground, yet were less likely to report bullying, and were more likely to report being happy at school.23 More research is needed, but these results suggest that children may be less intimidated by rough interactions when outdoor play involves loose parts.

The Millennium Cohort Study24 has also revealed some associations with independent outdoor play that initially appear counterintuitive. Three important findings are that children with higher levels of independent outdoor play were: less pro-social, more likely to be from lower socioeconomic status backgrounds and living in close proximity to both family friends and family. The authors speculate that children who are more pro-social may spend more time helping others rather than engaging in independent outdoor play. They also argue that parents of children from lower socioeconomic backgrounds may have different parenting styles, including allowing more independence, either by necessity or choice.  

Research Gaps

To date, critical studies to help establish causality have not taken place although their need is recognized.25-27 Studies vary their definition of outdoors and play, and outcome measures for the same constructs vary considerably. Infants and very young children have not been participants in higher quality studies.

Information on gender or other individual differences is limited. There is evidence that changes in physical activity associated with changes in the outdoor school play environment are different for boys and girls.28 There are also gender differences related to rough and tumble play29 and pretend play.30 It therefore seems likely that gender differences exist in the relationships between outdoor play, cognition and social development.

Not enough is known about the impact of natural environments in comparison to built environments. Available evidence suggests that natural environments provide children with more challenge31 and stress-buffering conditions32,33 that can be important for promoting cognitive and social development.

Conclusions

Although strong causal claims are not yet possible, there is enough evidence to recommend that children are offered opportunities for regular high-quality outdoor play. Ideally, outdoor play should involve changing conditions, such as found in nature, to encourage children to adapt and increase flexibility. It should allow for risk-taking and peer interactions. Opportunities for independent mobility appear to be important, particularly for school-aged children.

Implications for Parents, Services and Policy

As can be seen, research on outdoor play and its relationship to social and cognitive development has raised some unexpected findings. If a child is struggling with pre-academic tasks, engaging the child in more direct instruction may seem like common sense. Yet allowing the child time to engage in high quality, unstructured outdoor play is much more likely to support their academic and social skills by promoting development of EFs.  Children who have the lowest levels of EFs gain the most from these types of experiences.34 After examining the evidence, some schools have introduced LPP.35 Others have established ‘no homework’ policies and actively encourage outdoor play after school.36,37

References

  1. Waters J, Rekers A. Young Children’s Outdoor Play-Based Learning. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Brussoni M, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/outdoor-play/according-experts/young-childrens-outdoor-play-based-learning. Published May 2019. Accessed May 2, 2019.
  2. Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K, Singer DG. Why play = learning: a challenge for parents and educators. In: Singer DG, Golinkoff RM, Hirsh-Pasek K, eds. Play = learning: how play motivates and enhances children’s cognitive and social-emotional growth. New York, NY, US: Oxford University Press; 2006:3-12. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195304381.003.0001.
  3. Wyver S, Tranter P, Naughton G, Little H, Sandseter EBH, Bundy A. Ten ways to restrict children’s freedom to play: The problem of surplus safety. Contemporary Issues in Early Childhood. 2010;11(3):263-277.
  4. Woolley HE, Griffin E. Decreasing experiences of home range, outdoor spaces, activities and companions: changes across three generations in Sheffield in north England. Children’s Geography. 2015;13(6):677-691. doi:10.1080/14733285.2014.952186
  5. Burdette HL, Whitaker RC. Resurrecting free play in young children. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine. 2005;159(1):46-50. doi:10.1001/archpedi.159.1.46
  6. Kim KH. The creativity crisis: the decrease in creative thinking scores on the Torrance Tests of Creative Thinking. Creativity Research Journal. 2011;23(4):285-295. doi:10.1080/10400419.2011.627805
  7. Gray P. The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. American Journal of Play. 2011;4(4):443-463. 
  8. Bundy A, Engelen L, Wyver S, et al. Sydney Playground Project: A cluster-randomized trial to increase physical activity, play, and social skills. Journal of School Health. 2017;87(10):751-759. doi:10.1111/josh.12550
  9. Fjortoft I. The natural environment as a playground for children: the impact of outdoor play activities in pre-primary school children. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2001;29(2):111-117. doi:10.1023/A:1012576913074
  10. Rose KA, Morgan IG, Ip J, et al. Outdoor activity reduces the prevalence of myopia in children. Ophthalmology. 2008;115(8):1279-1285. doi:10.1016/j.ophtha.2007.12.019
  11. Whitebread D. Free play and children’s mental health. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health. 2017;1(3):167-169. doi:10.1016/S2352-4642(17)30092-5
  12. Moriguchi Y, Chevalier N, Zelazo PD. Editorial: Development of executive function during childhood. Frontiers in Psychology. 2016;7:6. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00006
  13. Hillman CH, Pontifex MB, Castelli DM, et al. Effects of the FITKids randomized controlled trial on executive control and brain function. Pediatrics. 2014;134(4):e1063-71. doi:10.1542/peds.2013-3219
  14. Diamond A, Ling DS. Aerobic-Exercise and resistance-training interventions have been among the least effective ways to improve executive functions of any method tried thus far. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. June 2018. [Epub ahead of print] doi:10.1016/J.DCN.2018.05.001
  15. Lillard AS, Hopkins EJ, Dore RA, Palmquist CM, Lerner MD, Smith ED. Concepts and theories, methods and reasons: Why do the children (pretend) play? Reply to Weisberg, Hirsh-Pasek, and Golinkoff (2013); Bergen (2013); and Walker and Gopnik (2013). Psychological Bulletin. 2013;139(1):49-52. doi:10.1037/a0030521
  16. Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. 2007;318(5855):1387-1388. doi:10.1126/science.1151148
  17. Fang J-T, Lin J-J. School travel modes and children’s spatial cognition. Urban Studies. 2017;54(7):1578-1600. doi:10.1177/0042098016630513
  18. Risotto A, Tonucci F. Freedom of movement and environmental knowledge in elementary school children. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2002;22(1-2):65-77. doi:10.1006/JEVP.2002.0243
  19. Kullman K. Transitional geographies: making mobile children. Social & Cultural Geography. 2010;11(8):829-846. doi:10.1080/14649365.2010.523839
  20. Armitage M. Play pods in schools: An independent evaluation (2006-2009). England: Playpeople; 2010.
  21. Moore R, Cosco N. Early Childhood Outdoor Play and Learning Spaces (ECOPALS): Achieving Design Quality. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Brussoni M, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. http://www.child-encyclopedia.com/outdoor-play/according-experts/early-childhood-outdoor-play-and-learning-spaces-ecopals-achieving. Published May 2019. Accessed May 2, 2019. 
  22. Gibson JL, Cornell M, Gill T. A systematic review of research into the impact of loose parts play on children’s cognitive, social and emotional development. School Mental Health. 2017;9(4):295-309. doi:10.1007/s12310-017-9220-9
  23. Farmer VL, Williams SM, Mann JI, Schofield G, McPhee JC, Taylor RW. Change of school playground environment on bullying: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics. 2017;139(5):e20163072. doi:10.1542/peds.2016-3072
  24. Aggio D, Gardner B, Roberts J, et al. Correlates of children’s independent outdoor play: Cross-sectional analyses from the Millennium Cohort Study. Preventive Medicine Reports. 2017;8:10-14. doi:10.1016/J.PMEDR.2017.07.007
  25. Watt P. Living in an oasis: middle-class disaffiliation and selective belonging in an English suburb. Environment and Planning A. 2009;41(12):2874-2892. doi:10.1068/a41120
  26. Broberg A, Kyttä M, Fagerholm N. Child-friendly urban structures: Bullerby revisited. Journal of Environmental Psychology. 2013;35:110-120. doi:10.1016/j.jenvp.2013.06.001
  27. Diamond A. Research that helps move us closer to a world where each child thrives. Research in Human Development. 2015;12(3-4):288-294. doi:10.1080/15427609.2015.1068034
  28. Engelen L, Bundy AC, Naughton G, et al. Increasing physical activity in young primary school children - it’s child’s play: A cluster randomised controlled trial. Preventive Medicine. 2013;56(5):319-325. doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.02.007
  29. Lindsey EW. Physical activity play and preschool children’s peer acceptance: distinctions between rough-and-tumble and exercise play. Early Education and Development. 2014;25(3):277-294. doi:10.1080/10409289.2014.890854
  30. Fung W, Cheng RW. Effect of school pretend play on preschoolers’ social competence in peer interactions: gender as a potential moderator. Early Childhood Education Journal. 2017;45(1):35-42. doi:10.1007/s10643-015-0760-z
  31. Kahn PH, Weiss T, Thea Weiss J. The importance of children interacting with big nature. Children, Youth and Environment. 2017;27(2):7-24.
  32. Mygind L, Stevenson M, Liebst L, et al. Stress response and cognitive performance modulation in classroom versus natural environments: a quasi-experimental pilot study with children. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2018;15(6):1098. doi:10.3390/ijerph15061098
  33. Sandseter EBH, Kennair LEO. Children’s risky play from an evolutionary perspective: the anti-phobic effects of thrilling experiences. Evolutionary Psychology. 2011;9(2):147470491100900220. doi:10.1177/147470491100900212
  34. Diamond A, Lee K. Interventions shown to aid executive function development in children 4 to 12 years old. Science. 2011;333(6045):959-964.
  35. Travers P. Loose parts play: Canberra primary school students revel in playgrounds filled with junk. ABC News. https://www.abc.net.au/news/2017-05-09/loose-parts-play-recycling-junk-into-creative-playgrounds/8506212. Published May 2017. Accessed April 30, 2019.
  36. Bramfield Park Primary School. School policies. http://bramfieldparkps.wa.edu.au/our-school-2/policies/. Published 2018. Accessed April 30, 2019.
  37. P.S. 116 Manhattan. Playtime, Downtime, Family time. http://www.ps116.org/pdf. Published 2018. Accessed April 30, 2019.

How to cite this article:

Wyver S. The Influence of Outdoor Play on Social and Cognitive Development. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Brussoni M, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. https://www.child-encyclopedia.com/outdoor-play/according-experts/influence-outdoor-play-social-and-cognitive-development. Published: May 2019. Accessed April 19, 2024.

Text copied to the clipboard ✓