Play-Fighting During Early Childhood and its Role in Preventing Later Chronic Aggression


University of Lethbridge, Canada
, Rev. ed.

PDF version

Introduction 

In the last decades, the opportunity for children to engage in freely occurring play has eroded due to an increase in structured activities (e.g., sports, music, dance lessons) and an increasing intolerance for anything that may be construed as aggression. Because of the risk of accidental injury or perceived opportunities for abusive contact, rough-and-tumble play (RTP) – which comprises both chasing and wrestling – has been the form of play most severely curtailed.1 In times past, when it was not suppressed, estimates of the amount of freely-chosen play to involve RTP in children, especially males, was about 10%.2 Given the concerns for children’s safety and the relatively infrequent engagement in RTP, it would seem sensible to ban it from their lives. However, a growing body of experimental evidence with laboratory animals suggests that banning RTP may be counterproductive. RTP provides young animals with the opportunity to finely tune their behaviour in a contextually relevant manner with peers and so modify the brain mechanisms that underpin social skills.3

What the Research Shows

Obviously, experimentally manipulating childhood experiences to test for the effects of play is not possible. Thus, the strongest experimental evidence comes from studies of laboratory rodents, especially rats; however, there is mounting evidence from studies of children that is consistent with the findings on rodents.

Play and the laboratory rat

Once weaned, young rats spend about an hour per day engaged in RTP. Depriving young rats of the opportunity to play over the juvenile period (akin to between 5-11 years of age for children) leads to a wide range of deficits, the core of which involve an inability to attenuate their emotional reaction to novel or frightening situations, and this is associated with social deficits. These deficits are seen in the play-deprived rats’ failings to coordinate their movements with those of a social partner – critical for successful sexual union – and in their misreading of social signals – critical to prevent social encounters from escalating into aggression. Crucial to emotional self-regulation and social skills is the ability of the prefrontal cortex (PFC) to exert executive control over the options available.4,5 Engagement in RTP leads to a modified release of chemical factors in the brain that influence growth, and changes the number, complexity, and, critically, the function, of the cells of the PFC. In the juvenile period, RTP has been shown to affect the development of the PFC, but socially reared rats, with normal experience of RTP, given damage to the PFC as adults, exhibit deficits in social behaviour like play-deprived rats with intact brains.6

Importantly, unlike earlier studies with rats that relied on complete social isolation during the juvenile period, in recent decades, various paradigms have been developed that selectively affects the opportunity for social play, enabling the resultant deficits to be linked to deficient RTP experience. In addition, different methods of assessing social competency across different strains of rats strongly suggest that this role of RTP is a species typical function of play.7,8,9 Thus, the causal link between RTP and social competency is well established in rats.6  The same association between juvenile RTP and social competency has been experimentally established in hamsters,10 and correlations for this association have been reported in non-human primates.11 Together, these findings indicate that the role of RTP in socio-cognitive development may be common across many mammals, including humans.

What is special about RTP?

For RTP to remain playful, it must be, at least to some degree, reciprocal. That is, partners must show the restraint necessary to prevent one of the participants from always gaining and maintaining the advantage. Also, RTP can be unpredictable and ambiguous. Participants cannot predict when or if they will lose control of the situation, or, indeed, how they will regain it, nor always be certain about their partners’ intentions. So, if one partner transgresses the playful rules by being more forceful than expected, the recipient must decide as to whether that partner is abusing the situation or has just been carried away by the exuberance of the moment.12 Thus, RTP creates an experiential context that taxes and trains the executive functions of the PFC.6

Research on children

Children that engage in more RTP tend to be better liked by peers, over consecutive years exhibit better social skills, and, overall, perform more effectively in the school setting with regard to academic performance.13 Although the PFC is not fully developed until the mid- to late-twenties, by exposing young children to playful situations that require the exercise of turn taking, executive function can be improved, which shows that the PFC is amenable to enhanced function even before it is fully mature.14,15 Non-physical play encounters that have many of the same properties as RTP can include exercises, such as asking two children to draw something together – they would thus have to negotiate what to draw, how to draw it and determine what each individual would contribute to the drawing. Such negotiations tax the function of the PFC, as does the monitoring necessary to make sure that the partner does not cheat. Indeed, failure to engage peers in social play has been shown to retard the development of executive functions.16

Implications

There are different degrees of involvement of social skills in different types of aggression.17 Lack of suitable social skill enhancement with associated emotional self-regulation could have a negative impact on aggression in at least three ways. First, as indicated by the animal experiments, play-impoverished children may misread social cues and so escalate to aggression. Second, as is also suggested by the animal literature, play impoverished children may have a smaller tool kit of options for convincing peers to cooperate, and so may resort to aggression to gain some operational advantage. Third, more specific to humans, poor adjustment to the school setting, failure to make friends and poor academic performance may lead to frustration-induced aggression.18 Finding ways that allow children to gain the experiences that are important from RTP, either through RTP itself, or activities that simulate core experiences from RTP, such as turn taking, may be important to offset later aggression.

References

  1. Baines E, Blatchford P. Children’s games and playground activities in school and their role in development. In: Pellegrini AD, ed. The Oxford Handbook of the Development of Play. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2011:260-283.
  2. Smith PK. Play fighting and real fighting. Perspectives on their relationship. In: Schmitt A, Atzwanger K, Grammar K, Schäfer K, eds. New aspects of human ethology. New York, NY: Plenum Press; 1997:47-64.
  3. Pellis SM, Pellis VC. The Playful Brain. Venturing to the limits of neuroscience. Oxford, UK: One world Press; 2009.
  4. Goldberg E. The executive brain. Frontal lobes and the civilized mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2001.
  5. Rempel-Clower NL. Role of orbitofrontal cortex connections in emotion. Annals of the New York Academy of Science. 2007;1121:72-86.
  6. Pellis SM, Pellis VC, Ham JR, Stark RA. Play fighting and the development of the social brain: The rat’s tale. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2023;145:105037.
  7. Bijlsmma A, Omrami A, Spoelder M, Veharen JPH, Bauer L, Cornelis C, de Zwart B, van Dorland R, Vanderschuren LJ. MJ, Weirenga C. Social play behavior is critical for the development of prefrontal inhibitory synapses and cognitive flexibility. Journal of Neuroscience. 2022;42(46):8716-8728.
  8. Ham JR, Szabo M, Annor-Bediako J, Stark RA, Iwaniuk AN, Pellis SM. Quality not quantity: Deficient juvenile social experiences lead to altered medial prefrontal neurons and socio-cognitive skill deficiencies. Developmental Psychobiology. 2024;66(2):e22456.
  9. Schneider P, Bindila L, Schmahl C, Bohus M, Meyer-Lindenberg A, Lutz B, Spanagel R, Schneider M. Adverse social experience, pain sensitivity and endocannabinoid signalling. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. 2016;10:203.
  10. Cooper AA, Grizzell JA, Whitten CJ, Burghardt GM. Comparing the ontogeny, neurobiology, and functions of social play in hamsters and rats. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2023;147:105102.
  11. Palagi E. Not just for fun! Social play as a springboard for adult social competence in human and non-human primates. Behavioural Ecology and Sociobiology. 2018;72(6):90.
  12. Pellis SM, Pellis VC, Ham, JR. Play fighting revisited: Its design features and how they shape our understanding of its mechanisms and functions. Frontiers in Ethology. 2024; 3:1362052.
  13. Pellegrini AD. The role of play in human development. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2009.
  14. Diamond A, Barnett WS, Thomas J, Munro S. Preschool program improves cognitive control. Science. 2007;318(5855):1387-1388.
  15. Gibb R, Coelho L, Van Rootselaar NA, Halliwell C, MacKinnon M, Plomp I, Gonzalez CLR. Promoting executive function skills in preschoolers using a play-based program. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12:720225.
  16. Nijhof SL, Vinkers CH, van Geelen SM, Duijff SN, Achterberg EJM, van der Net J, Veltkamp RC, Grootenhuis MA, van de Putte EM, Hillegers MHJ, van der Brug AW, Wierenga CJ, Benders MJNL, Engels RCME, van der Ent CK, Vanderschuren LJMJ, Lesscher HMB. Healthy play, better coping: The importance of play for the development of children in health and disease. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews. 2018;95:421-429.
  17. Kaukiainen A, Björkqvist K, Lagerspetz K, Österman K, Salmivalli C, Rothberg S, Ahlbom A. The relationships between social intelligence, empathy, and three types of aggression. Aggressive Behavior. 1999;25:81-89.
  18. Renfrew JR. Aggression and its causes: A biopsychosocial approach. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 1997.
     

How to cite this article:

Pellis SM, Pellis VC, Ham JR. Play-Fighting During Early Childhood and its Role in Preventing Later Chronic Aggression. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Tremblay RE, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. https://www.child-encyclopedia.com/aggression/according-experts/play-fighting-during-early-childhood-and-its-role-preventing-later. Updated: May 2025. Accessed May 20, 2025.

Text copied to the clipboard ✓