[Archived] Prenatal/Perinatal Stress and Its Impact on Psychosocial Child Development
Janet DiPietro, PhD
Johns Hopkins University, USA
, Rev. ed.
Introduction
From time immemorial, there have been stories about the effect of a mother’s emotions on a developing foetus. The belief that a foetus may be harmed by negative maternal emotions related to stress and anxiety has its roots in cultural tradition and folklore that span the globe. A significant amount of scientific inquiry has attempted to determine the validity of such beliefs by examining the manner in which maternal stress and anxiety may be related to pregnancy outcomes and postnatal development in children.1-3
Subject
It has been purported that maternal stress during pregnancy acts as a developmental teratogen (a substance that can negatively affect development) as can be the case with drugs or alcohol. Articles in magazines and newspapers often perpetuate this belief, thus (ironically) exacerbating stress in pregnant women, who may be unable to control difficult life circumstances. This report will provide a review of evidence in the debate regarding whether or not maternal stress during pregnancy causes poor developmental outcomes in children.
Problems
Measuring and defining stress is a complicated undertaking. A woman’s appraisal of whether something is stressful is affected by many factors, including aspects of her personality, such as her anxiety level and outlook on life. Objectively speaking, a woman who reports a great deal of stress during pregnancy may not be experiencing a more stressful time than any other woman. She may simply be more susceptible than other women to forceful negative reactions when confronted with daily hassles. Women who report feeling stressed, anxious or depressed during pregnancy also report feeling this way through at least their child’s second year of life.4 It is therefore difficult to distinguish stress from other psychological or personality characteristics. In addition, it is difficult to separate effects of maternal stress that are the result of biological changes to the developing foetus from effects imposed by postnatal differences in parenting by mothers who are stressed.
Research Context
A woman’s thoughts are not transmitted to the foetus because there are no direct neural connections between them. However, maternal stress and emotions produce a cascade of hormonal reactions, changes in blood flow to the uterus, and other alterations that directly influence the intrauterine milieu. In fact, given the intricate physiological relationship between mother and foetus, it would be somewhat surprising if dynamic aspects of the maternal environment did not serve to shape foetal development. The key word here is “shape,” not “damage.” Studies of animals (primarily rodents and non-human primates) allow experimental manipulation of prenatal stress in a manner in way that humans cannot. Findings from animal studies often contradict one another, with some demonstrating that prenatal stress impairs development while others show that it facilitates adaptative developmental functioning.
Key Research Questions
Here we pose the following questions regarding prenatal influences in human pregnancies on postnatal developmental outcomes:
- Do maternal stress and negative emotions contribute to preterm delivery and low birthweight which may, in turn, have ramifications for development?
- Do maternal stress and negative emotions directly affect the foetal nervous system, thereby resulting in alterations in cognitive, behavioural or emotional development after birth?
Recent Research Results
The answer to the first question is a qualified “no.” That is, while some, but not all, studies detect an association between prenatal stress exposure and smaller babies or earlier deliveries,5-7 the magnitude of effects when detected are small and unlikely to negatively influence subsequent child development. This includes studies that capitalize on “experiments in nature” to evaluate whether stressful events that affect a population, as opposed to personality characteristics, are associated with negative outcomes.8-9 A meta-analysis, which is a statistical review of all studies on a topic, concluded that maternal anxiety, which is highly comorbid with maternal stress, was unrelated to pregnancy outcomes.10
The second question has been informed most predominantly by studies that rely on maternal report of child developmental outcomes. However, because women who are anxious or stressed view their children as more difficult or worrisome, such studies cannot provide valid information about this relationship. Outcomes of some studies that did directly measure child development include a link between prenatal stress or anxiety and slower maturation in infancy or early childhood and reduced attention levels.11-13 However, others have found the opposite: that prenatal psychological distress is associated with faster neural and behavioural maturation.14-15 In general, the relatively few existing studies that measure child outcomes report sporadic and inconsistent associations of relatively small magnitude.
Studies that purport a link between maternal stress and subsequent psychiatric disorders should be equally interpreted with caution due to the inability to ascertain the roles of shared genetic contribution and the postnatal rearing environment. For example, while there is a well-established relationship between prenatal cigarette use and childhood ADHD, this association is explained primarily by inherited traits’16 that is, women who smoke cigarettes are different from women who do not in a manner that is consistent with more prevalent ADHD symptoms in their offspring. The same may be likely for prenatal stress exposure.
Research Gaps
The most apparent research gap is the paucity of empirical studies on the topic that:
- measure features of child developmental outcomes, as opposed to relying on maternal reports about child difficulties which are directly contaminated by maternal psychological functioning, and
- adequately control for maternal stress in the postnatal period.
In addition, there is a lack of large-scale studies that include exposed and non-exposed siblings to allow appropriate evaluation of the role of inheritance and rearing in significant findings.
Conclusions
While there is some evidence that negative prenatal maternal emotions or experiences result in smaller or earlier delivered infants, results across studies are neither uniform nor compelling. Of those that detect associations, there is some evidence suggesting that earlier, as opposed to later exposures, may be more influential. However, the magnitude of effects, when detected, is small and unlikely to independently contribute to negative child developmental outcomes.
There is not nearly enough quality data to determine that prenatal maternal stress either benefits or damages child development and as such, it should not be regarded as a developmental teratogen.
Perhaps the most promising and newer line of research is to reduce the emphasis on the construct of maternal psychological stress during pregnancy and instead focus on examining associations between neuroendocrine and physiological parameters during pregnancy in relation to child outcomes. In particular, products of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis such as cortisol, which manages the body’s response to stress, has engendered increasing attention. Contrary to expectations, there is little to no correspondence between cortisol and maternal stress or other negative emotional states during pregnancy.12,13 Shifting the focus from psychological constructs to biological ones may be more productive in understanding complex associations. There is some existing evidence that variation in maternal cortisol levels, independent of psychological stress, has consequences for child development, although again, some studies find facilitative effects while others find negative ones.12,17
Implications
Faced with insufficient and conflicting scientific data, pregnant women and their providers should adopt a common-sense approach to managing prenatal stress and determining when stress should be mitigated. The assumption that “stress is bad, so stress will hurt my baby” is unjustified. Worrying about one’s level of stress or anxiety is clearly counter-productive.18 In fact, it is evident from studies of both animals and humans that moderate, but not overwhelming, stress can facilitate development. Ultimately, our understanding of the relationship between a mother’s psychological state and development in the foetus she carries will likely reveal complex relations with regard to child psychosocial development. Public policy issues in this area of research would include maternal employment and maternity leave. In point of fact, it is not uncommon for pregnant women to work until the time of birth. We hardly need research on the role of prenatal maternal stress in child development to deduce that this may not be an optimal societal expectation. Women who work at demanding, stressful jobs throughout pregnancy may enter labour, delivery, and the ensuing years of child dependency with depleted energy reserves. Furthermore, the implications of maternal stress on the postnatal environment that is created for the infant are likely to be of far greater consequence than potential biological effects of prenatal exposures. Nonetheless, because it is up to each woman to subjectively appraise whether an event or circumstance is stressful, public policy to govern the behaviour or activities of pregnant women in the interest of improving child developmental outcomes should not be established.
References
- Dunkel-Schetter C, Wadhwa P, Stanton AL. Stress and reproduction: Introduction to the special section. Health Psychology 2000;19(6):507‑509.
- Paarlberg KM, Vingerhoets AD JJM, Passchier J, Dekker GA, Geijn Hv. Psychosocial factors and pregnancy outcome: a review with emphasis on methodological issues. Journal of Psychosomatic Research 1995;39(5):563‑595.
- van den Bergh RH, Mulder EJH, Mennes M, Glover V. Antenatal maternal anxiety and stress and the neurobehavioral development of the fetus and child: links and possible mechanisms. A review. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 2005;29;237-258.
- DiPietro JA, Costigan K, Sipsma H. Continuity in self-report measures of maternal anxiety, stress, and depressive symptoms from pregnancy through two years postpartum. Journal of Psychosomatic Obstetrics and Gynecology 2008;29:115-124.
- Copper RL, Goldenberg RL, Das A, Elder N, Swain M, Norman G, Ramsey R, Cotroneo P, Collins BA, Johnson F, Jones P, Meier AM. The preterm prediction study: Maternal stress is associated with spontaneous preterm birth at less than thirty-five weeks' gestation. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology 1996;175(5):1286‑1292.
- Wadhwa PD, Sandman CA, Porto M, Dunkel-Schetter C, Garite TJ. The association between prenatal stress and infant birth weight and gestational age at birth: A prospective investigation. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology 1993;169(4):858‑865.
- Rice F, Harold GT, Boivin J, van den Bree M, Hay DF, Thapar A. The links between prenatal stress and offspring development and psychopathology: disentangling environmental and inherited influences. Psychological Medicine 2010;40;335-345.
- Lipkind HS, Curry AE, Huynh M, Thorpe LE, Matte T. Birth outcomes among offspring of women exposed to the September 11, 2001, terrorist attacks. Obstetrics & Gynecology 2010;116;917-925.
- Glynn LM, Wadhwa PD, Dunkel-Schetter C, Chicz-DeMet A, Sandman CA. When stress happens matters: effects of earthquake timing on stress responsivity in pregnancy. American Journal of Obstetrics & Gynecology 2001;184:637-642.
- Littleton H, Breitkopf C, Berenson A. Correlates of anxiety symptoms during pregnancy and association with perinatal outcomes: A meta-analysis. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 2007;196:424-432.
- Huizink AC, Robles de medina PG, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA, Buitelaar JK. Stress during pregnancy is associated with developmental outcome in infancy. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry 2003;44:810-818.
- Davis EP, Sandman CA. The timing of prenatal exposure to maternal cortisol and psychosocial stress is associated with human infant cognitive development. Child Development 2010:81;131-148.
- Gutteling BM, de Weerth C, Zandbelt N, Mulder EJH, Visser GHA, Buitelaar JK. Does maternal prenatal stress adversely affect the child’s learning and memory at age six? Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology 2006:34;789-798.
- DiPietro JA, Novak MF, Costigan KA, Atella LD, Reusing SP. Maternal psychological distress during pregnancy in relation to child development at age 2. Child Development 2006;77;573-587.
- DiPietro JA, Kivlighan KT, Costigan KA, Rubin SE, Shiffler DE, Henderson JL, Pillion JP. Prenatal antecedents of newborn neurological maturation. Child Development 2010;81:115-130.
- Lindblad F, Hjern A. ADHD after fetal exposure to maternal smoking. Nicotine & Tobacco Research 2010;4:408-415.
- LeWinn KZ, Stroud LR, Molnar BE, Ware JH, Koenen KC, Buka SL. Elevanted maternal cortisol levels during pregnancy are associated with reduced childhood IQ. International Journal of Epidemiology 2009;38:1700-1710.
- Oates MR. Adverse effects of maternal antenatal anxiety on children: causal effect or developmental continuum? British Journal of Psychiatry 2002;180:478-479.
How to cite this article:
DiPietro J. [Archived] Prenatal/Perinatal Stress and Its Impact on Psychosocial Child Development. In: Tremblay RE, Boivin M, Peters RDeV, eds. Glover V, topic ed. Encyclopedia on Early Childhood Development [online]. https://www.child-encyclopedia.com/stress-and-pregnancy-prenatal-and-perinatal/according-experts/prenatalperinatal-stress-and-its. Updated: January 2011. Accessed October 8, 2024.
Text copied to the clipboard ✓